Hard Truths About Race on Campus

Saturday, May 14th, 2016

Jonathan Haidt and Lee Jussim address some hard truths about race on campus:

A basic principle of psychology is that people pay more attention to information that predicts important outcomes in their lives. A key social factor that we human beings track is who is “us” and who is “them.” In classic studies, researchers divided people into groups based on arbitrary factors such as a coin toss. They found that, even with such trivial distinctions, people discriminated in favor of their in-group members.

None of this means that we are doomed to discriminate by race. A 2001 study by Robert Kurzban of the University of Pennsylvania and colleagues in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences found that race was much less prominent in how people categorized each other when individuals also shared some other prominent social characteristic, like membership on a team. If you set things up so that race conveys less important information than some other salient factor, then people pay less attention to race.

A second principle of psychology is the power of cooperation. When groups face a common threat or challenge, it tends to dissolve enmity and create a mind-set of “one for all, all for one.” Conversely, when groups are put into competition with each other, people readily shift into zero-sum thinking and hostility.

[...]

But as practiced in most of the top American universities, affirmative action also involves using different admissions standards for applicants of different races, which automatically creates differences in academic readiness and achievement. Although these gaps vary from college to college, studies have found that Asian students enter with combined math/verbal SAT scores on the order of 80 points higher than white students and 200 points higher than black students. A similar pattern occurs for high-school grades. These differences are large, and they matter: High-school grades and SAT scores predict later success as measured by college grades and graduation rates.

As a result of these disparate admissions standards, many students spend four years in a social environment where race conveys useful information about the academic capacity of their peers. People notice useful social cues, and one of the strongest causes of stereotypes is exposure to real group differences. If a school commits to doubling the number of black students, it will have to reach deeper into its pool of black applicants, admitting those with weaker qualifications, particularly if most other schools are doing the same thing. This is likely to make racial gaps larger, which would strengthen the negative stereotypes that students of color find when they arrive on campus.

And racial gaps in classroom performance create other problems. A 2013 study by the economist Peter Arcidiacono of Duke University found that students tend to befriend those who are similar to themselves in academic achievement. This is a big contributor to the patterns of racial and ethnic self-segregation visible on many campuses. If a school increases its affirmative-action efforts in ways that expand these gaps, it is likely to end up with more self-segregation and fewer cross-race friendships, and therefore with even stronger feelings of alienation among black students.

[...]

In their book All That We Can Be (1996), the sociologists Charles Moskos and John Sibley Butler describe how the U.S. Army escaped from the racial dysfunction of the 1970s to become a model of integration and near-equality by the time of the 1991 Gulf War. The Army invested more resources in training and mentoring black soldiers so that they could meet rigorous promotion standards. But, crucially, standards were lowered for no one, so that the race of officers conveyed no information about their abilities. The Army also promoted cooperation and positive-sum thinking by emphasizing pride in the Army and in America.

Comments

  1. Bob Sykes says:

    If you go over to CDR Salamander, you will read in his Diversity Thursday series the numerous ways in which the SJWs in the military have lowered standards for selected groups.

  2. Kirk says:

    Having served in the Army from 1982 to 2007, all I can say about that last paragraph, after laughing my ass off hysterically, is “Bullshit”.

    All they did was paper over the cracks, and establish a two-tier system where competency counted only for whites and other non-protected minorities. If you had the right “qualifications”, like being one of the few “Asian-Pacific Islanders” in a given career field, you could damn near get away with raping the Colonel’s daughter on the conference table, and still get selected by your “unbiased” promotion board.

    Racism is alive and well, in the US Army. All that’s changed is the color of the victim’s skin. Merit and competency play almost no role in determining who gets promoted–It’s all about the percentages. You’re a white boy with mediocre-looking records? You’re never seeing E-7. Black guy? Hispanic? Asian-Pacific Islander? Same sort of records? You just need to keep breathing, brother–You’ll hit below the zone, every damn time. Watched it happen–I know there were minority NCOs with significant black marks like relief for cause NCOERs, DWI, all kinds of shit, and they all got promoted by the centralized boards. Meanwhile, the white guys who didn’t have that shit in their records? Nope, no rank for you.

    You knew the fix was in for this shit when they did away with the Skills Qualification Testing back in the early ’90s. Right now, there is literally nothing in the Army system that either encourages or checks for actual verified competency in your basic MOS skills. Nobody cares–It’s all about how good you look in a uniform, how well you can march troops, and what color your skin is.

    Mark my words–There will be a comeuppance, and likely, damn soon. Wonder why we just came in last, in that little tank gunnery competition in Europe? Yeah… Chickens coming home to roost, baby. Hollow Army? Yeah… Empty Army, more like it.

  3. Slovenian Guest says:

    Kirk is referring to the Strong Europe Tank Challenge 2016:

    A recent competition hosted in part by the U.S. Army and designed to test core tank crew skills saw European crews take the top honors, while crews from the U.S. Army failed to place. The results raise the question of whether the Army—after more than a decade of focusing on guerrilla warfare—has devoted adequate training to address “big war” skills.

    Held from May 10 to 12 and jointly hosted by the U.S. Army and the German Bundeswher at the Grafenwoehr Training Area in Germany, the the Strong Europe Tank Challenge included challengers from six NATO countries: Denmark, Germany, Italy, Poland, and Slovenia—which sent tank platoons of four tanks each to compete— and the United States, which sent two platoons.

    The competition involved tank crews conducting both offensive and defensive operations, and both mounted and dismounted activities. Crews fired ten main gun rounds from various positions. In one event, crews had to correctly identify 25 friendly and unfriendly (read: Russian) vehicles while traveling a course. Other events involved operating in the aftermath of a chemical weapons attack, dealing with improvised explosive devices, and medical emergencies.

    A German tank crew from Mountain Panzer Battalion 7, Panzer Brigade 12 took top honors, followed by a Danish crew from their country’s 1st Tank Battalion in second. Third place went to a Polish crew from the 34th Armored Cavalry brigade. It’s unknown where the American crews placed, only that they weren’t in the top three.

    Epic fail! via Popular Mechanics

Leave a Reply