A Rime of Ice atop a Sea of Habit

Tuesday, June 2nd, 2015

Julian Jaynes explored the nature of consciousness and came up with an unusual explanation:

To explore the origins of this inner country, Jaynes first presents a masterful precis of what consciousness is not. It is not an innate property of matter. It is not merely the process of learning. It is not, strangely enough, required for a number of rather complex processes. Conscious focus is required to learn to put together puzzles or execute a tennis serve or even play the piano. But after a skill is mastered, it recedes below the horizon into the fuzzy world of the unconscious. Thinking about it makes it harder to do. As Jaynes saw it, a great deal of what is happening to you right now does not seem to be part of your consciousness until your attention is drawn to it. Could you feel the chair pressing against your back a moment ago? Or do you only feel it now, now that you have asked yourself that question?

Consciousness, Jaynes tells readers, in a passage that can be seen as a challenge to future students of philosophy and cognitive science, “is a much smaller part of our mental life than we are conscious of, because we cannot be conscious of what we are not conscious of.” His illustration of his point is quite wonderful. “It is like asking a flashlight in a dark room to search around for something that does not have any light shining upon it. The flashlight, since there is light in whatever direction it turns, would have to conclude that there is light everywhere. And so consciousness can seem to pervade all mentality when actually it does not.”

Perhaps most striking to Jaynes, though, is that knowledge and even creative epiphanies appear to us without our control. You can tell which water glass is the heavier of a pair without any conscious thought — you just know, once you pick them up. And in the case of problem-solving, creative or otherwise, we give our minds the information we need to work through, but we are helpless to force an answer. Instead it comes to us later, in the shower or on a walk. Jaynes told a neighbor that his theory finally gelled while he was watching ice moving on the St. John River. Something that we are not aware of does the work.

The picture Jaynes paints is that consciousness is only a very thin rime of ice atop a sea of habit, instinct, or some other process that is capable of taking care of much more than we tend to give it credit for. “If our reasonings have been correct,” he writes, “it is perfectly possible that there could have existed a race of men who spoke, judged, reasoned, solved problems, indeed did most of the things that we do, but were not conscious at all.”

Jaynes believes that language needed to exist before what he has defined as consciousness was possible. So he decides to read early texts, including The Iliad and The Odyssey, to look for signs of people who aren’t capable of introspection — people who are all sea, no rime. And he believes he sees that in The Iliad. He writes that the characters in The Iliad do not look inward, and they take no independent initiative. They only do what is suggested by the gods. When something needs to happen, a god appears and speaks. Without these voices, the heroes would stand frozen on the beaches of Troy, like puppets.

Speech was already known to be localized in the left hemisphere, instead of spread out over both hemispheres. Jaynes suggests that the right hemisphere’s lack of language capacity is because it used to be used for something else — specifically, it was the source of admonitory messages funneled to the speech centers on the left side of the brain. These manifested themselves as hallucinations that helped guide humans through situations that required complex responses — decisions of statecraft, for instance, or whether to go on a risky journey.

The combination of instinct and voices — that is, the bicameral mind — would have allowed humans to manage for quite some time, as long as their societies were rigidly hierarchical, Jaynes writes. But about 3,000 years ago, stress from overpopulation, natural disasters, and wars overwhelmed the voices’ rather limited capabilities. At that point, in the breakdown of the bicameral mind, bits and pieces of the conscious mind would have come to awareness, as the voices mostly died away. That led to a more flexible, though more existentially daunting, way of coping with the decisions of everyday life — one better suited to the chaos that ensued when the gods went silent. By The Odyssey, the characters are capable of something like interior thought, he says. The modern mind, with its internal narrative and longing for direction from a higher power, appear.

Comments

  1. Grasspunk says:

    Under his theory, would there be bicameral folk running around today?

  2. Alex J. says:

    3,000 years is not that long ago, evolutionarily speaking. Native Americans, Aboriginal Australians, Andaman Islanders, Bushmen, etc. would not show changes that happened that recently. Perhaps Jaynes’ suggested changes are not genetic, and a modern person raised in an ancient environment would have a bicameral mind again?

    (Oh, to study the Sentinelese…)

  3. Jaynes’ theory is entirely unrelated to genetic changes, Alex. You are correct, though, that one of its weaknesses is that there are no humans left with bicameral minds despite many groups not having undergone the societal and environmental changes that he posits were its undoing.

  4. Eli says:

    How about comparing animals? Wild animals that “know” when to migrate, where to migrate to? What route to take? Domesticated animals do not show this trait, nor do F1 feral animals. I can think of Canadian geese that needed to be shown how though, so maybe not?

  5. R. says:

    Jaynes’ theory is entirely unrelated to genetic changes, Alex. You are correct, though, that one of its weaknesses is that there are no humans left with bicameral minds despite many groups not having undergone the societal and environmental changes that he posits were its undoing.

    It's not. He posits that genes played a role in its disappearance.

    Also, it could be argued that the bicameral mind is a liability even for hunter-gatherers, so one can imagine it being on retreat for quite some time everywhere.

    Also, telling apart a bicameral person from a normal one would involve a lot of questioning in the person's native language. Not sure if this kind of thing's ever been attempted.

    There is something weird going on with the human mind. About 50% of people admit to hearing voices at least once, and children have imaginary friends. I thought these were, well, imaginary, but if you stick kids conversing with imaginary friends into an fMRI machine, they show brain activity like people having auditory hallucinations.

  6. Spandrell says:

    His theory of language also didn’t work out. He thought language was very recent, but Bushmen are 200k years apart from the rest of humanity, and they speak perfectly well.

    I could buy that schizophrenia was more prevalent than today; and that schizophrenics had higher status because they could speak with the gods, so they set up the whole priest-kinghood thing, and people followed.

    I remember perfectly well my imaginary friends at 5 years old. I couldn’t actually hear it. fMRI shows activation all over the brain when processing language, can’t reach conclusions from that.

    All pre-state farming societies are organized in large tribes of common descent. Which means the ancestors are a big deal. Claiming to be able to hear old dad’s voice carries a lot of weight; so it’s no wonder people said they could. Motivated reasoning goes a long way. Men actually claim to be feminist these days.

Leave a Reply